Otras publicaciones:

9789877230284-frontcover

Book cover

Otras publicaciones:

saenz-gabini_frontcover1

isaacson

Referencias bibliográficas

Adamson, L. B., y McArthur, D. (1995). Joint attention, affect, and culture. In C. MooreP. J. Dunham (Eds.), Joint attention: Its origins and role in development (pp. 205-221). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

Anderson, D. R., y Pempek, T. A. (2005). Television and very young children. American Behavioral Scientist, 48(5), 505-522.

Aschersleben, G. (2006). Early development of action control. Psychology Science, 48(4), 405-418.

Baldwin, D., Andersson, A., Saffran, J., y Meyer, M. (2008). Segmenting dynamic human action via statistical structure. Cognition, 106(3), 1382-1407.

Baldwin, D., Baird, J. A., Saylor, M. M., y Clark, M. A. (2001). Infants parse dynamic action. Child Development, 72(3), 708-717.

Barnett, S. M., y Ceci, S. J. (2002). When and where do we apply what we learn? A taxonomy for far transfer. Psychological Bulletin, 128(4), 612-623.

Barr, R. (2008). Attention to and learning from media during infancy and early childhood. In S. L. Calvert y B. J. Wilson (Eds.), Blackwell handbook of child development and the media (pp. 143-165). Malden, MA: Blackwell.

Barr, R. (2010). Transfer of learning between 2D and 3D sources during infancy: Informing theory and practice. Developmental Review, 30(2), 128-154.

Barr, R. (2013). Memory constraints on infant learning from picture books, television, and touchscreens. Child Development Perspectives, 7(4), 205-210.

Barr, R., Dowden, A., y Hayne, H. (1996). Developmental changes in deferred imitation by 6-to 24-month-old infants. Infant Behavior and Development, 19(2), 159-170.

Barr, R., y Hayne, H. (1999). Developmental changes in imitation from television during infancy. Child Development, 70(5), 1067-1081.

Barr, R., Zack, E., García, A., y Muentener, P. (2008). Infants’ attention and responsiveness to television increases with prior exposure and parental interaction. Infancy, 13(1), 30-56.

Barresi, J. y Moore, C. (1996). Intentional relations and social understanding. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 19(1), 107-122.

Behne, T., Carpenter, M. y Tomasello, M. (2005). One‐year‐olds comprehend the communicative intentions behind gestures in a hiding game. Developmental Science, 8(6), 492-499.

Beilin, H. (1983). Development of photogenic comprehension. Art Education, 36(2), 28-33.

Bergen, D., Hutchinson, K., Nolan, J. T. y Weber, D. (2009). Effects of infant-parent play with a technology-enhanced toy: Affordance-related actions and communicative interactions. Journal of Research in Childhood Education, 24(1), 1-17.

Biro, S., y Leslie, A. M. (2007). Infants’ perception of goal‐directed actions: development through cue‐based bootstrapping. Developmental Science, 10(3), 379-398.

Blass, E. M., Fillion, T. J., Rochat, P., Hoffmeyer, L. B., y Metzger, M. A. (1989). Sensorimotor and motivational determinants of hand-mouth coordination in 1-3-day old human infants. Developmental Psychology, 25(6), 963-975.

Bloom, P., y Markson, L. (1998). Intention and analogy in children’s naming of pictorial representations. Psychological Science, 9(3), 200-204.

Byrne, R. W. (1996). Relating brain size to intelligence in primates. In P. A. Mellars y K. R. Gibson (Eds.), Modelling the Early Human Mind (pp. 49-56). Cambridge, UK: MacDonald Institute for Archaeological Research.

Callaghan, T. C. (2000). Factors affecting children’s graphic symbol use in the third year: Language, similarity, and iconicity. Cognitive Development, 15(2), 185-214.

Callaghan, T. C. (2005). Developing an intention to communicate through drawing. Enfance, 57(1), 45-56.

Callaghan, T. C., Rochat, P., MacGillivray, T., y MacLellan, C. (2004). Modeling Referential Actions in 6‐to 18‐Month‐Old Infants: A Precursor to Symbolic Understanding. Child Development, 75(6), 1733-1744.

Campbell, D. T. y Stanley, J. C. (1963). Experimental and quasi-experimental designs for research on teaching. En N. L. Gage (Ed.), Handbook of research on teaching (pp. 171-246). Chicago, IL: Rand McNally.

Cannon, E. N. y Woodward, A. L. (March, 2010). Familiar actions trump “action-effects” in goal detection in the first year. Paper presented at International Conference on Infant Studies. Baltimore, MD.

Cannon, E. N., y Woodward, A. L. (2012). Infants generate goal‐based action predictions. Developmental Science, 15(2), 292-298.

Carpenter, M., Akhtar, N. y Tomasello, M. (1998). Fourteen-through 18-month-old infants differentially imitate intentional and accidental actions. Infant Behavior and Development, 21(2), 315-330.

Carpenter, M., Nagell, K., Tomasello, M., Butterworth, G., y Moore, C. (1998). Social cognition, joint attention, and communicative competence from 9 to 15 months of age. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, i-174.

Chen, Z., y Klahr, D. (2008). Remote transfer of scientific-reasoning and problem-solving strategies in children. Advances in Child Development and Behavior, 36, 419-470.

Chen, Z., y Siegler, R. S. (2013). Young children’s analogical problem solving: Gaining insights from video displays. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 116(4), 904-913.

Chiong, C., y Shuler, C. (2010). Learning: Is there an app for that? Investigations of young children’s usage and learning with mobile devices and apps. New York: The Joan Ganz Cooney Center at Sesame Workshop. Retrieved from https://goo.gl/31Q5Mq

Choi, K. y Kirkorian, H. L. (2016). Touch or watch to learn? Toddlers’ object retrieval using contingent and noncontingent video. Psychological Science, 27(5), 726-736.

Christie, S., y Gentner, D. (2010). Where hypotheses come from: Learning new relations by structural alignment. Journal of Cognition and Development, 11(3), 356-373.

Clark, H. H. (1996). Using language. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

Coll, C. (2004). Psicología de la educación y prácticas educativas mediadas por las tecnologías de la información y la comunicación. Una mirada constructivista. Sinéctica, Revista Electrónica de Educación, 25, 1-24. Recuperado de https://goo.gl/tMiGnZ

Couse, L. J., y Chen, D. W. (2010). A Tablet computer for young children? Exploring its viability for early childhood education. Journal of Research on Technology in Education, 43(1), 75-96.

Csibra, G. (2008). Action mirroring and action understanding: An alternative account. In P. Haggard, Y. Rosetti y M. Kawato (Eds.), Sensorimotor Foundations of Higher Cognition. Attention and Performance, XXII (pp. 435-459). Oxford: Oxford University Press.

Csibra, G. (2010). Recognizing communicative intentions in infancy. Mind & Language, 25(2), 141-168.

Csibra, G., Biró, S., Koós, O., y Gergely, G. (2003). One-year-old infants use teleological representations of actions productively. Cognitive Science, 27(1), 111-133.

Csibra, G., y Gergely, G. (2009). Natural pedagogy. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 13(4), 148-153.

Dalke, D. E. (1998). Charting the development of representational skills: When do children know that maps can lead and mislead? Cognitive Development, 13(1), 53-72.

Daum, M. M., y Gredebäck, G. (2011). The development of grasping comprehension in infancy: Covert shifts of attention caused by referential actions. Experimental Brain Research, 208(2), 297-307.

Daum, M. M., Prinz, W., y Aschersleben, G. (2011). Perception and production of object-related grasping in 6-month-olds. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 108(4), 810-818.

DeLoache, J. S. (1987). Rapid change in the symbolic functioning of very young children. Science, 238(4833), 1556-1557.

DeLoache, J. S. (1991). Symbolic functioning in very young children: Understanding of pictures and models. Child Development, 62(4), 736-752.

DeLoache, J. S. (1995). Early symbol understanding and use. Psychology of Learning and Motivation, 33, 65-116.

DeLoache, J. S. (2000). Dual representation and young children’s use of scale models. Child Development, 71(2), 329-338.

DeLoache, J. S. (2002). The symbol-mindedness of young children. In W. Hartup y R. A. Weinberg (Eds.), Child psychology in retrospect and prospect: In celebration of the 75th anniversary of the Institute of Child Development (Vol. 32, pp. 73-101). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc.

DeLoache, J. S. (2004). Becoming symbol-minded. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 8(2), 66-70.

DeLoache, J. S., y Burns, N. M. (1994). Early understanding of the representational function of pictures. Cognition, 52(2), 83-110.

DeLoache, J. S., y DeMendoza, O. A. (1987). Joint picturebook interactions of mothers and 1‐year‐old children. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 5(2), 111-123.

DeLoache, J. S. y Marzolf, D. P. (1992). When a picture is not worth a thousand words: Young children’s understanding of pictures and models. Cognitive Development, 7, 317-329.

DeLoache, J. S., Miller, K. F., y Rosengren, K. S. (1997). The credible shrinking room: Very young children’s performance with symbolic and nonsymbolic relations. Psychological Science, 8(4), 308-313.

DeLoache, J. S., Pierroutsakos, S. L., y Uttal, D. H. (2003). The origins of pictorial competence. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 12(4), 114-118.

DeLoache, J. S., Uttal, D. H., y Pierroutsakos, S. L. (1998). The development of early symbolization: Educational implications. Learning and Instruction, 8(4), 325-339.

Dennett, D. C. (1987). The intentional stance. Cambridge, MA: Bradford Books/MIT Press.

Detterman, D. K., y Sternberg, R. J. (1993). Transfer on trial: Intelligence, cognition, and instruction. Westport, CT: Ablex Publishing.

Donald, M. (1991). Origins of the modern mind: Three stages in the evolution of culture and cognition. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

Donald, M. (2000). The central role of culture in cognitive evolution: A reflection on the myth of the isolated mind. In L. P. Nucci, G. B. Saxe and E. Turiel (Eds.), Culture, Thought, and Development (pp. 19-38). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

Duit, R. (1991). On the role of analogies and metaphors in learning science. Science Education, 75(6), 649-672.

Falck-Ytter, T., Gredebäck, G., y Von Hofsten, C. (2006). Infants predict other people’s action goals. Nature Neuroscience, 9(7), 878-879.

Ferguson, B., Graf, E., y Waxman, S. R. (2014). Infants use known verbs to learn novel nouns: Evidence from 15-and 19-month-olds. Cognition, 131(1), 139-146.

Ferry, A. L., Hespos, S. J., y Waxman, S. R. (2010). Categorization in 3- and 4-month-old infants: An advantage of words over tones. Child Development, 81, 472-479. Recuperado de: https://goo.gl/UgfdbG

Flanagan, J. R. y Johansson, R. S. (2003). Action plans used in action observation. Nature, 424(6950), 769-771.

Fletcher, K. L. y Reese, E. (2005). Picture book reading with young children: A conceptual framework. Developmental Review, 25(1), 64-103.

Fulkerson, A. L., y Waxman, S. R. (2007). Words (but not tones) facilitate object categorization: Evidence from 6-and 12-month-olds. Cognition, 105(1), 218-228.

Gallese, V., Rochat, M., Cossu, G., y Sinigaglia, C. (2009). Motor cognition and its role in the phylogeny and ontogeny of action understanding. Developmental Psychology, 45(1), 103.

Ganea, P. A., Pickard, M. B., y DeLoache, J. S. (2008). Transfer between picture books and the real world by very young children. Journal of Cognition and Development, 9(1), 46-66.

Ganea, P. A., Allen, M. L., Butler, L., Carey, S., y DeLoache, J. S. (2009). Toddlers’ referential understanding of pictures. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 104(3), 283-295.

Gentner, D., Anggoro, F. K., y Klibanoff, R. S. (2011). Structure mapping and relational language support children’s learning of relational categories. Child Development, 82(4), 1173-1188.

Gentner, D., Holyoak, K. J., y Kokinov, B. N. (2001). The analogical mind: Perspectives from cognitive science. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

Gentner, D. y Markman, A. B. (1997). Structure mapping in analogy and similarity. American Psychologist, 52(1), 45.

Gentner, D., y Namy, L. L. (1999). Comparison in the development of categories. Cognitive Development, 14(4), 487-513.

Gergely, G. y Csibra, G. (2003). Teleological reasoning in infancy: The naive theory of rational action. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 7, 287-292.

Gerson, S., y Woodward, A. L. (2010). Building intentional action knowledge with one’s hands. In S Johnson (Ed.), Neoconstructivism: The new science of cognitive development (pp. 295-313). New York: Oxford University Press.

Gerson, S., y Woodward, A. L. (2012). A claw is like my hand: comparison supports goal analysis in infants. Cognition, 122(2), 181-192.

Gerson, S., y Woodward, A. L. (2013). The goal trumps the means: Cueing goals is more beneficial than cueing means in means-end training. Infancy, 18(2), 289-302.

Gerson, S. y Woodward, A. L. (2014a). Learning from their own actions: the unique effect of producing actions on infants’ action understanding. Child Development, 85(1), 264-277.

Gerson, S. y Woodward, A. L. (2014b). Labels facilitate infants’ comparison of action goals. Journal of Cognition and Development, 15(2), 197-212.

Gómez, C. B., Sanjosé, V., y Solaz-Portolés, J. J. (2012). Una revisión de los procesos de transferencia para el aprendizaje y enseñanza de las ciencias. Didáctica de las Ciencias Experimentales y Sociales, 26, 199-227. Recuperado de: https://goo.gl/hYvkge

Gopnik, A., Meltzoff, A. N., y Bryant, P. (1997). Words, thoughts, and theories (Vol.1). Cambridge, MA: Mit Press.

Grezes, J., y Decety, J. (2001). Functional anatomy of execution, mental simulation, observation, and verb generation of actions: a meta‐analysis. Human Brain Mapping, 12(1), 1-19.

Hamlin, J. K, Hallinan, E. V, y Woodward, A. L. (2008). Do as I do: 7-month-old infants selectively reproduce others’ goals. Developmental Science, 11(4), 487-94.

Harris, P. L., Kavanaugh, R. D. y Dowson, L. (1997). The depiction of imaginary transformations: Early comprehension of a symbolic function. Cognitive Development, 12(1), 1-19.

Hauf, P., y Aschersleben, G. (2008). Action-effect anticipation in infant action control. Psychological Research, 72(2), 203-210.

Heider, F., y Simmel, M. (1944). An experimental study of apparent behavior. The American Journal of Psychology, 57(2), 243-259.

Hernik, M., y Southgate, V. (2012). Nine‐months‐old infants do not need to know what the agent prefers in order to reason about its goals: On the role of preference and persistence in infants’ goal‐attribution. Developmental Science, 15(5), 714-722.

Hofer, T., Hauf, P., y Aschersleben G. Infant’s perception of goal-directed actions performed by a mechanical device. Infant Behavior and Development, 28(4), 466-480.

Hommel, B., Müsseler, J., Aschersleben, G., y Prinz, W. (2001). Codes and their vicissitudes. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 24(5), 910-926.

Hunnius, S. y Bekkering, H. (2014). What are you doing? How active and observational experience shape infants’ action understanding. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci, 369(1644), 20130490.

Ittelson, W. H. (1996). Visual perception of markings. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 3(2), 171-187.

Jauck, D., y Peralta, O. (2016). La comprensión simbólica temprana de una imagen digital como medio de comunicación y fuente de información. Anales de Psicología, 32(3), 886-892.

Jovanovic, B., Király, I., Elsner, B., Gergely, G., Prinz, W. y Aschersleben, G. (2007). The role of effects for infants’ perception of action goals. Psychologia, 50(4), 273-290.

Kanakogi, Y. y Itakura, S. (2011). Developmental correspondence between action prediction and motor ability in early infancy. Nature Communications, 2, 341.

Karmiloff-Smith, A. (1994). Más allá de la modularidad: la ciencia cognitiva desde la perspectiva del desarrollo. Madrid: Alianza.

Kellman, P. J. y Spelke, E. S. (1981). Infant perception of partly occluded objects: Sensitivity to movement and configuration. In Biennial meetings of the Society for Research in Child Development, Boston.

Király, I., Jovanovic, B., Prinz, W., Aschersleben, G., y Gergely, G. (2003). The early origins of goal attribution in infancy. Consciousness and Cognition, 12(4), 752-769.

Kirkham, N. Z., Slemmer, J. A., y Johnson, S. P. (2002). Visual statistical learning in infancy: Evidence for a domain general learning mechanism. Cognition, 83(2), B35-B42.

Kirkorian, H. L., Wartella, E. A., y Anderson, D. R. (2008). Media and young children’s learning. The Future of Children, 18(1), 39-61.

Kirkorian, H. L., y Pempek, T. A. (2013). Toddlers and touch screens: Potential for early learning. Zero to Three, 33(4), 32-37.

Kirkorian, H. L., Choi, K., y Pempek, T. A. (2016). Toddlers’ word learning from contingent and noncontingent video on touch screens. Child Development, 87(2), 405-413.

Klahr, D., y Chen, Z. (2011). Finding one’s place in transfer space. Child Development Perspectives, 5(3), 196-204.

Kochukhova, O., y Gredebäck, G. (2010). Preverbal infants anticipate that food will be brought to the mouth: An eye tracking study of manual feeding and flying spoons. Child Development, 81(6), 1729-1738.

Krcmar, M. (2010). Can social meaningfulness and repeat exposure help infants and toddlers overcome the video deficit? Media Psychology, 13(1), 31-53.

Lauricella, A. R., Pempek, T. A., Barr, R., y Calvert, S. L. (2010). Contingent computer interactions for young children’s object retrieval success. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology, 31(5), 362-369.

Leslie, A. M. (1995). A theory of agency. En D. Sperber, D. Premack y A. J., Premack (Eds.), Causal Cognition: A Multidisciplinary Debate (pp. 121-141). Oxford, UK: Clarendon Press.

Lillard, A. S. (1993). Young children’s conceptualization of pretense: Action or mental representational state? Child Development, 64(2), 372-386.

Lisenbee, P. (2009). Whiteboards and web sites: Digital tools for the early childhood curriculum. YC Young Children, 64(6), 92.

Lovato, S. B. y Waxman, S. R. (2016). Young Children Learning from Touch Screens: Taking a Wider View. Frontiers in Psychology, 7, 1078.

Lowenstein, J., y Gentner, D. (2001). Spatial mapping in preschoolers: Close comparisons facilitate far mappings. Journal of Cognition and Development, 2(2), 189-219.

Luo, Y. y Baillargeon, R. (2005). Can a self-propelled box have a goal? Psychological reasoning in 5-month-old infants. Psychological Science, 16(8), 601-608.

Llegada de un tren a la estación de La Ciotat. (1985). [Video] Francia: Auguste Marie Louis Nicolas Lumière y Louis Jean Lumière.

MacKenzie, H., Graham, S. A., y Curtin, S. (2011). Twelve‐month‐olds privilege words over other linguistic sounds in an associative learning task. Developmental Science, 14(2), 249-255.

Maita, M. R. y Peralta, O. A. (2007). La comprensión infantil de objetos simbólicos: un verdadero desafío cognitivo. Revista Intercontinental de Psicología y Educación, 9(2), 163-180.

Maita, M. D. R., Mareovich, F. y Peralta, O. (2014). Intentional Teaching Facilitates Young Children’s Comprehension and Use of a Symbolic Object. The Journal of Genetic Psychology, 175(5), 401-415.

Martí, E. (2003). Representar el mundo externamente. Madrid: Antonio Machado Libros.

Martí, E. (2012). Desarrollo del pensamiento e instrumentos culturales. En J. Castorina y M. Carretero (comps.), Desarrollo cognitivo y educación II. Procesos del conocimiento y contenidos específicos, (pp. 25-44). Buenos Aires: Paidós.

Martí, E. y Pozo, J. I. (2000). Más allá de las representaciones mentales: la adquisición de los sistemas externos de representación. Infancia y Aprendizaje, 23(90), 11-30.

Meltzoff, A. N. (1988). Imitation of televised models by infants. Child Development, 59(5), 1221-1229.

Meltzoff, A. N. (1995). Understanding the intentions of others: re-enactment of intended acts by 18-month-old children. Developmental Psychology, 31(5), 838-850.

Meltzoff, A. N. (1999). Origins of theory of mind, cognition and communication. Journal of Communication Disorders, 32(4), 251-269.

Meltzoff, A. N. (2002). Elements of a Developmental Theory of Imitation. In A. N. Meltzoff y W. Prinz (Eds.), The Imitative Mind: Development, Evolution, and Brain Bases (pp. 19-41). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.

Meltzoff, A. N. (2007). ‘Like me’: a foundation for social cognition. Developmental Science, 10(1), 126-134.

Meltzoff, A. N. y Borton, R. W. (1979). Intermodal matching by human neonates. Nature, 282(5737), 403-404.

Meltzoff, A. N., y Moore, M. K. (1983). Newborn infants imitate adult facial gestures. Child Development, 54(3), 702-709.

Meltzoff, A. N., y Moore, M. K. (1997). Explaining facial imitation: A theoretical model. Early Development and Parenting, 6(3-4): 179-192.

Moore, R., Liebal, K. y Tomasello, M. (2013). Three-year-olds understand communicative intentions without language, gestures, or gaze. Interaction Studies, 14(1), 62-80.

Namy, L. L., y Gentner, D. (2002). Making a silk purse out of two sow’s ears: Young children’s use of comparison in category learning. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 131(1), 5.

Namy, L. y Waxman, S. (2005). Symbol Redefined. En L. Namy (Ed.), Symbolic use and understanding. Mahwah, N. J.: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates Publishers.

Ninio, A., y Bruner, J. (1978). The achievement and antecedents of labeling. Journal of Child Language, 5(1), 1-15.

Olson, D. R. (1994). The world on paper: The conceptual and cognitive implications of writing and reading. New York: Cambridge University Press.

Onishi K. H., y Baillargeon, R. (2005). Do 15-month-old infants understand false beliefs? Science, 308(5719), 255-8.

O’Sullivan, L. P., Mitchell, L. L., y Daehler, M. W. (2001). Representation and perseveration: Influences on young children’s representational insight. Journal of Cognition and Development, 2(4), 339-365.

Peralta, O. A. (1995). Developmental changes and socioeconomic differences in mother-infant picturebook reading. European Journal of Psychology of Education, 10(3), 261-272.

Peralta, O., y DeLoache, J. (2004). La comprensión y el uso de fotografías como representaciones simbólicas por parte de niños pequeños. Infancia y Aprendizaje, 27(1), 3-14.

Peralta, O. A., y Salsa, A. M. (2001). Interacción materno-infantil con libros con imágenes en dos niveles socioeconómicos. Infancia y Aprendizaje, 24(3), 325-339.

Peralta, O. A., y Salsa, A. M. (2009). Means of Communication and Sources of Information: Two-Year-Old Children’s Use of Pictures as Symbols. The European Journal of Cognition and Development, 21(6), 801-812.

Peralta, O. A., y Salsa, A. M. (2011). Instrucción y desarrollo en la comprensión temprana de fotografías como objetos simbólicos. Anales de Psicología, 27(1), 118-125.

Perner, J. (1991). Understanding the Representational Mind. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

Piaget, J. (1952). The origins of intelligence in children (Vol. 8, No. 5, pp. 18-1952). New York: International Universities Press.

Pruden, S. M., Hirsh‐Pasek, K., Golinkoff, R. M., y Hennon, E. A. (2006). The Birth of Words: Ten‐Month‐Olds Learn Words Through Perceptual Salience. Child Development, 77(2), 266-280.

Pozo, J. I. (2003). Adquisición de conocimiento: cuando la carne se hace verbo. Madrid: Morata.

Pierroutsakos, S. L. y Troseth, G. L. (2003, April). Video verite: Infant’s manual investigation of objects on video. Infant Behavior and Development, 26(2), 183-199.

Premack, D. y Premack, A. J. (1995). Origins of human social competence. In M. S. Gazzaniga (Ed.), The Cognitive Neurosciences. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

Preissler, M. y Carey, S. (2004). Do Both Pictures and Words Function as Symbols for 18- and 24-month-old Children? Journal of Cognition and Development, 5(2), 185-212.

Prinz, W. (1997). Perception and action planning. European Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 9(2), 129-154.

Rakoczy, H., Tomasello, M., y Striano, T. (2005). On tools and toys: how children learn to act on and pretend with ‘virgin objects’. Developmental Science, 8(1), 57-73.

Ramsey, R., y Hamilton, A. F. (2010). How does your own knowledge influence the perception of another person’s action in the human brain? SCAN, 7, 242-251.

Ratterman, M. J., y Gentner, D. (1998). More evidence for a relational shift in the development of analogy: Children’s performance on a casual-mapping task. Cognitive Development, 13(4), 453-478.

Richert, R. A., Robb, M. B., y Smith, E. I. (2011). Media as social partners: The social nature of young children’s learning from screen media. Child Development, 82(1), 82-95.

Rideout, V., y Hamel, E. (2006). The media family: Electronic media in the lives of infants, toddlers, preschoolers and their parents. Menlo Park, CA: Kaiser Family Foundation. Recuperado de https://goo.gl/cXlnns

Rideout, V., Wartella, E. y Lauricella, A. (2011). Children, media, and race: Media use among White, Black, Hispanic, and Asian American children. Report for the Center on Media and Human Development. Evanston, IL: Northwestern University. Recuperado de: https://goo.gl/v6Gs7f

Rivière, Á. (1990). Origen y desarrollo de la función simbólica en el niño. En J. Palacios, A. Marchesi y C. Coll (Eds.), Desarrollo psicológico y educación, Vol. I, Psicología evolutiva (pp. 113-130). Madrid: Alianza Editorial.

Rivière, Á. y Sotillo, M. (2003). Comunicación, suspensión y semiosis humana: los orígenes de la práctica y de la comprensión interpersonales. En M. Belinchón, A. Rosa, M. Sotillo, y I. Marichalar (comps.) Ángel Rivière. Obras Escogidas, Vol III (pp. 181-201). Madrid: Panamericana.

Rochat, P. (1998). Self-perception and action infancy. Experimental Brain Research, 123(1-2), 102-109.

Rochat, P. (2004). El mundo del bebé (Vol. 27). Madrid: Morata.

Roseberry, S., Hirsh-Pasek, K., y Golinkoff, R. M. (2014) Skype me! Socially contingent interactions help toddlers learn language. Child Development, 85(3), 956-70.

Sánchez, A. R. (2015). Los niños menores de tres años y la televisión: perspectivas de investigación y debate (1999-2010). Revista Luciérnaga-Comunicación, 4(7), 12-22.

Salsa, A. M., y De Mendoza, O. A. P. (2003). Un modelo para el estudio del desarrollo de la comprensión y el uso de símbolos en niños pequeños. Interdisciplinaria, 20(1), 75-98.

Salsa, A. M., y Peralta, O. A. (2007). Routes to symbolization: Intentionality and correspondence in early understanding of pictures. Journal of Cognition and Development, 8(1), 79-92.

Saylor, M. M., Baldwin, D. A., Baird, J. A., y LaBounty, J. (2007).Infants’ on-line segmentation of dynamic human action. Journal of Cognition and Development, 8(1), 113-128.

Schmitt, K. L., y Anderson, D. R. (2002). Television and reality: Toddlers’ use of visual information from video to guide behavior. Media Psychology, 4(1), 51-76.

Schmidt, M. E., Crawley-Davis, A. M., y Anderson, D. R. (2007). Two-year-olds’ object retrieval based on television: Testing a perceptual account. Media Psychology, 9(2), 389-409.

Seehagen, S., y Herbert, J. S. (2010). The role of demonstrator familiarity and language cues on infant imitation from television. Infant Behavior and Development, 33(2), 168-175.

Sharon, T. (2005). Made to symbolize Intentionality and children early understanding of symbols. Journal of Cognition and Development, 6(2), 163-178.

Sharon, T., y DeLoache, J. S. (2003). The role of perseveration in children’s symbolic understanding and skill. Developmental Science, 6(3), 289-296.

Sigel, I. (1978). The development of pictorial comprehension. En B. Randhawa y W. Coffman (Eds.), Visual learning, thinking and communication (pp. 93-111). Nueva York: Academic Press.

Simcock, G., y DeLoache, J. S. (2006). The effects of iconicity on re-enactment from picture books by 18-to 30-month-old children. Developmental Psychology, 42(6), 1352-1357.

Simcock, G., Garrity, K., y Barr, R. (2011). The effect of narrative cues on infants’ imitation from television and picture books. Child Development, 82(5), 1607-1619.

Singley, M. K., y Anderson, J. R. (1989). The transfer of cognitive skill (N° 9). Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

Slater, A., Quinn, P. C., Brown, E., y Hayes, R. (1999). Intermodal perception at birth: Intersensory redundancy guides newborn infants’ learning of arbitrary auditory-visual pairings. Developmental Science, 2(3), 333-338.

Snow, C. E., y Goldfield, B. A. (1983). Turn the page please: Situation-specific language acquisition. Journal of Child Language, 10(3), 551-569.

Sommerville, J. A., Hildebrand, E. A., y Crane, C. C. (2008). Experience matters: the impact of doing versus watching on infants’ subsequent perception of tool-use events. Developmental Psychology, 44(5), 1249.

Sommerville, J. A., Woodward, A. L., y Needham, A. (2005). Action experience alters 3-month-old infants’ perception of others’ actions. Cognition, 96(1), B1-B11.

Southgate, V., y Csibra, G. (2009). Inferring the outcome of an ongoing novel action at 13 months. Developmental Psychology, 45(6), 1794.

Spelke, E. S., Phillips, A. y Woodward, A. L. (1995). Infants’ knowledge of object motion and human action. In D. Sperber, D. Premack y A. J. Premack (Eds.), Symposia of the Fyssen Foundation. Causal cognition: A multidisciplinary debate (pp. 44-78). New York: Clarendon Press/Oxford University Press.

Spelke, E. S., Vishton, P., y Von Hofsten, C. (1995). Object perception, object-directed action, and physical knowledge in infancy. In M. Gazzaniga (Ed.), The Cognitive Neurosciences. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

Strouse, G. A., y Ganea, P. A. (2016). Are Prompts Provided by Electronic Books as Effective for Teaching Preschoolers a Biological Concept as Those Provided by Adults? Early Education and Development, 27(8), 1190-1204.

Strouse, G. A., y Troseth, G. L. (2008). Don’t try this at home: Toddlers’ imitation of new skills from people on video. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 101(4), 262-280.

Stapel, J. C., Hunnius, S., Van Elk, M., y Bekkering, H. (2010). Motor activation during observation of unusual versus ordinary actions in infancy. Social Neuroscience, 5(5-6), 451-460.

Stern, D. N. (1985). The interpersonal world of the infant: A view from psychoanalysis and developmental psychology. London: Karnac Books.

Suddendorf, T. (2003). Early Representational Insight: Twenty‐four‐month‐olds can use a photo to find an object in the world. Child Development, 74(3), 896-904.

Tare, M., Chiong, C., Ganea, P. y DeLoache, J. (2010). Less is more: How manipulative features affect children’s learning from picture books. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology, 31(5), 395-400.

Teubal, E. (2010). The contribution of non-verbal graphic texts to children’s early literacy development. IRICE, 21, 27-36.

Tolchinsky, L. (1993). Aprendizaje del lenguaje escrito. Barcelona: Anthropos.

Tomasello, M. (1995). Joint attention as social cognition. En C. Moore y P. Dunham (Eds.), Joint attention: Its origins and role in development (pp. 103- 30). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.

Tomasello, M. (1999). The Cultural Origins of Human Cognition. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

Tomassello, M. (2000). The Cultural Origins of Human Cognition. London: Harvard Universtiy Press.

Tomasello, M. (2008). Origins of Human Communication. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

Tomasello, M. (2009). Why We Cooperate. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

Tomasello, M., y Akhtar, N. (1995). Two-year-olds use pragmatic cues to differentiate reference to objects and actions. Cognitive Development, 10(2), 201-224.

Tomasello, M., y Carpenter, M. (2007). Shared intentionality. Developmental Science, 10(1), 121-125.

Tomasello, M., Carpenter, M., Call, J., Behne, T., y Moll, H. (2005) Understanding and sharing intentions: The origins of cultural cognition. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 28, 675-735.

Troseth, G. (2003). TV guide: Two-year-old children learn to use video as a source of information. Developmental Psychology, 39(1), 140-150.

Troseth, G. L., Russo, C. E., y Strouse, G. A. (2016). What’s next for research on young children’s interactive media? Journal of Children and Media, 10(1), 54-62.

Troseth, G. L. y DeLoache, J. S. (1998). The medium can obscure the message: Young children’s understanding of video. Child Development, 69(4), 950-965.

Troseth, G. L., Saylor, M. M., y Archer, A. H. (2006). Young children’s use of video as a source of socially relevant information. Child Development, 77(3), 786-799.

Uttal, D. H., O’Doherty, K., Newland, R., Hand, L. L. y DeLoache, J. (2009). Dual representation and the linking of concrete and symbolic representations. Child Development Perspectives, 3(3), 156-159.

Vygotsky, L. S (1978). Mind in society: The development of higher psychological processes. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

Vygotsky, L. S. (1979). El desarrollo de los procesos psíquicos superiores. Barcelona: Crítica.

Vygotsky, L. S. (1996). Obras escogidas, 4. Psicología infantil. Madrid: Visor.

Von Hofsten, C. (1990). Development of manipulation action in infancy. En H. Bloch y B. I. Berthental (Eds.), Sensory-motor organizations and development in infancy and early childhood (pp. 33-43). Dordrecht: Kluwer.

Von Hofsten, C., y Lindhagen, K. (1979). Observations on the development of reaching for moving objects. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 28(1), 158-173.

Vouloumanos, A., y Waxman, S. R. (2014). Listen up! Speech is for thinking during infancy. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 18(12), 642-646.

Walsh, E. (2005). Los niños pequeños y los aparatos electrónicos. Berkeley, California: Childcare Health Program.

Warneken, F., Chen, F., y Tomasello, M. (2006). Cooperative activities in young children and chimpanzees. Child Development, 77(3), 640-663.

Warneken, F. y Tomasello, M. (2006). Altruistic helping in human infants and young chimpanzees. Science, 311(5765), 1301-1303.

Warneken, F. y Tomasello, M. (2007). Helping and cooperation at 14 months of age. Infancy, 11(3), 271-294.

Waxman, S. R. (1991). Contemporary approaches to concept development. Cognitive Development, 6(1), 105-118.

Waxman, S. R. (2001). Word extension: A key to early word learning and domain-specificity. Commentary on P. Bloom, Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 24(6), 1121-1122.

Waxman, S. R. (2008). All in good time: How do infants discover distinct types of words and map them to distinct kinds of meaning? In J. Colombo, P. McCardle y L. Freund (Eds.), Infant pathways to language: Methods, models, and research directions (pp. 99-118). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

Waxman, S. R., y Booth, A. E. (2001). Seeing pink elephants: Fourteen-month-olds’ interpretations of novel nouns and adjectives. Cognitive Psychology, 43(3), 217-242.

Waxman, S. R., y Booth, A. E. (2003). The origins and evolution of links between word learning and conceptual organization: New evidence from 11-month-olds. Developmental Science, 6(2), p 130-137.

Waxman, S. R., y Goswami, U. (2012). Learning about language and literacy. In S. Pauen y M. Bornstein (Eds.), Early childhood development and later achievement. London: Cambridge University Press.

Waxman, S. R., y Lidz, J. (2006). Early word learning. In D. Kuhn y R. Siegler (Eds.), Handbook of child psychology, 6th Edition, Vol. 2 (pp. 299-335). Hoboken NJ: Wiley.

Woodward, A. L. (1998). Infants selectively encode the goal object of an actor’s reach. Cognition, 69(1), 1-34.

Woodward, A. L. (2003). Infants’ developing understanding of the link between looker and object. Developmental Science, 6(3), 297-311.

Woodward, A. L. (2009). Infants’ grasp of others’ intentions. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 18(1), 53-57.

Woodward, A. L. (2013). Infant foundations of intentional understanding. In M. R. Banaji y S. A. Gelman (Eds.), Navigating the Social World: A Developmental Perspective (pp.75-80). Oxford: Oxford University Press.

Woodward, A. L., Sommerville, J. A., y Guajardo, J. J. (2001). How infants make sense of intentional action. In B. Malle, L. Moses y D. Baldwin (Eds.), Intentions and intentionality: Foundations of Social Cognition (pp.149-169). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

Woodward, A. L., y Guajardo, J. J. (2002). Infants’ understanding of the point gesture as an object-directed action. Cognitive Development, 17(1), 1061-1084.

Woodward, A. L., Sommerville, J., Gerson, S. A., Henderson, A. M. E. y Buresh, J. (2009). The emergence of intention attribution in infancy. En B. Ross (Ed.), The Psychology of Learning and Motivation (pp. 187-222). Waltham, MA: Academic Press.

Woodward, A. L., y Gerson, S. A. (2014). Mirroring and the development of action understanding. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci, 369, 20130181.

Zack, E., Barr, R., Gerhardstein, P., Dickerson, K., y Meltzoff, A. N. (2009). Infant imitation from television using novel touch screen technology. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 27(1), 13-26.

Zimmerman, F. J., Christakis, D. A., y Meltzoff, A. N. (2007). Associations between media viewing and language development in children under age 2 years. The Journal of Pediatrics, 151(4), 364-368.



Deja un comentario